Attention:
it’s one of the most debated topics in psychological research, yet it is
arguably impossible to find an environment where it is absent. Even in this
instance, your body is not only paying attention to the words on this page, but
it is also paying attention to the temperature of the room, the oxygen levels
of the air, and the ground beneath you. However, these are not directed forms
of attention. Directed attention occurs when you focus on something, such as
learning Spanish or watching Family Guy. But how do people specifically do
this? And more interestingly, why do some people show deficits and differences
in terms of how they pay attention, and how much attention they give, to
certain things? This question has been partially answered with the
establishment of certain disorders such as attention deficit disorder and attention
hyperactive deficit disorder. In Frank McCormick’s poem, “Attention Deficit
Disorder”, he tries to capture the very essence of the disorder. One thing that
I want to highlight in this post is the fact that I believe that the naming for
ADD is incredibly inaccurate, and I believe that McCormick may agree with me.
His poem starts out as follows:
I guess I paid attention to the
wrong things:/
the creamy empty space between the
words,/
the silent edges of the textbook
illustration,/
the worn lines along my teacher’s
thick brown neck./
Still, I could re-draw them in my
sleep.
This
first stanza of the poem highlights a very interesting, important feature of
ADD, in my opinion. This is the fact that the word “deficit” can be very
deceiving and almost untruthful about the overall attention state of those who
have ADD. People with ADD do not suffer from a strict deficit of attention, per say. From this stanza, I would say that
McCormick argues that it is more of an altercation or reallocation of
attention. The narrator says “I guess I paid attention to the wrong things”. So
it does not seem like he does not pay attention, but rather that he believes
his attention was on things aside from what he was specifically supposed to be
paying attention to. It seems safe to assume the narrator is a school student.
He goes on to refer to the “creamy empty space between the words” and the “silent
edges of the textbook illustration”. It seems as if the narrator is paying
attention to his school book, but not what normal people without ADD would most
likely pay attention to. Most of us don’t spend time focusing our attention on
the white space between the text, or the edges of pictures, but this caught the
narrator’s eye for some reason. The most interesting line from this stanza,
however, is the last one. He states that he could draw the “worn lines along my
teacher’s thick brown neck…in my sleep”. No one would argue that the wrinkles
on the teacher’s neck is a very particular detail. To pick up on such a feature
requires extreme attention. But hold on….this is has attention deficit disorder. Is he truly capable of such detailed
memory for something when he was clearly not paying attention to the lecture
material. Well, it seems that McCormick would argue “Yes,” and I would have to
agree. I believe that the name Attention Deficit Disorder is extremely
deceiving, and it makes you wonder if the disorder deserves a more fitting
name, such as Attention Distribution Disorder.
Furthermore, I believe that N. Katherine
Hayles has some very interesting contributions to this argument in her paper, “Hyper
and Deep Attention: The Generational Divide in Cognitive Modes”. In this paper, Hayles addresses the ideas of
deep attention and hyper attention. She states that “deep attention is
superb for solving complex problems represented in a single medium, but it
comes at the price of environmental alertness and flexibility of response” (p.
188) This seems to relate perfectly to the situation in McCormick’s poem, “Attention
Deficit Disorder”, where the narrator says he is able to remember the teacher’s
neck lines. In this situation, the narrator’s deep attention was focused on the
teacher’s neck, and he was oblivious to the environment around him. Hayles also
states that “hyper attention excels at negotiating rapidly changing
environments in which multiple foci compete for attention; its disadvantage is
impatience with focusing for long periods on a noninteractive object such as a
Victorian novel or complicated math problem” (p. 188). This highlights the
narrator’s inability to pay attention to the words on the page of his textbook.
However, the teacher, who was a much more interactive object to hold the
narrator’s attention, became the object of focus, and the vivid memory of the
neck lines was the result. This displays the very intriguing allocation of
attention that, I would argue, is in no way a deficit, but more of a
disruption.
No comments:
Post a Comment